NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Location
California1
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 16 to 30 of 32 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zentall, Thomas R.; Stagner, Jessica P. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Pigeons show a preference for an alternative that provides them with discriminative stimuli (sometimes a stimulus that predicts reinforcement and at other times a stimulus that predicts the absence of reinforcement) over an alternative that provides them with nondiscriminative stimuli, even if the nondiscriminative stimulus alternative is…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Behavior, Reinforcement, Probability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Szucs, Denes; Soltesz, Fruzsina; Bryce, Donna; Whitebread, David – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2009
The ability to select an appropriate motor response by resolving competition among alternative responses plays a major role in cognitive performance. fMRI studies suggest that the development of this skill is related to the maturation of the frontal cortex that underlies the improvement of motor inhibition abilities. However, fMRI cannot…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Competition, Child Development, Motor Reactions
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Schumacher, Susan J. – Teaching of Psychology, 1982
A laboratory project introduced physiological psychology students to research by requiring them to identify an unknown drug given to laboratory animals. Students read material about drugs and animal drug studies, designed behavioral tests, constructed the testing apparatus, conducted the tests, and wrote progress reports. (SR)
Descriptors: Drug Use, Higher Education, Laboratory Animals, Laboratory Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stagner, Jessica P.; Laude, Jennifer R.; Zentall, Thomas R. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
When pigeons are given a choice between two alternatives, one leading to a stimulus 20% of the time that always signals reinforcement (S+) or another stimulus 80% of the time that signals no reinforcement (S-), and the other alternative leading to one of two stimuli each signaling reinforcement 50% of the time, they show a strong preference for…
Descriptors: Animals, Reinforcement, Probability, Stimuli
National Association of School Nurses (NJ1), 2012
This article presents position statements, issue briefs, and resolutions and consensus statements of the National Association of School Nurses (NASN). The Position Statements include: (1) Allergy/Anaphylaxis Management in the School Setting; (2) Caseload Assignments; (3) Child Mortality in the School Setting; (4) Chronic Health Conditions, Managed…
Descriptors: Sexuality, Child Abuse, Violence, Health Programs
Kramm, Kenneth R. – Humane Education, 1982
Describes an alternative method for using live animals in the classroom. A toy dog, the "Trail Tracker Hound Dog" (manufactured by CPG Products Corporation, Cincinnati, Ohio), is used to encourage development of such skills as observation, hypothesis testing, and collection and analysis of scientific data. (Author/JN)
Descriptors: Animal Behavior, Animals, Elementary Education, Elementary School Science
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Yonas, Albert – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2001
Comments on Needham's research of infant perception by focusing on the types of evidence needed to make inferences concerning infant cognition. Considers the history of scientific explanations of animal cognition as nearer to infant cognition, and the high level of creativity required in proposing and testing alternative explanations of infant…
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Associative Learning, Cognitive Development, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Carlin, Joel L. – Bioscene: Journal of College Biology Teaching, 2010
Dissections of single species (e.g., fetal pig) are a common student learning activity in introductory biology courses. Such dissections demonstrate location of anatomical parts and provide dissection practice but provide less opportunity for student critical thinking, numeracy and demonstration of the scientific method. A comparative anatomy lab…
Descriptors: Scientific Methodology, Laboratory Procedures, Biology, Anatomy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Glazer, Howard I.; Weiss, Jay M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976
Presents three experiments that explore whether inescapable shock of long duration and moderate intensity (LoShk) produces an avoidance-escape deficit (called an interference effect) by causing animals to learn to respond less actively or by causing them to learn to be "helpless". (Editor)
Descriptors: Animal Behavior, Behavior Patterns, Charts, Experimental Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kogan, C. S.; Boutet, I.; Cornish, K.; Graham, G. E.; Berry-Kravis, E.; Drouin, A.; Milgram, N. W. – Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 2009
Background: Standardised neuropsychological and cognitive measures present some limitations in their applicability and generalisability to individuals with intellectual disability (ID). Alternative approaches to defining the cognitive signatures of various forms of ID are needed to advance our understanding of the profiles of strengths and…
Descriptors: Mental Retardation, Down Syndrome, Short Term Memory, Recognition (Psychology)
National Academy of Sciences - National Research Council, Washington, DC. Inst. of Lab. Animal Resources. – 1991
The Committee on Education Programs in Laboratory Animal Science (EPLAS) has prepared this guide to aid institutions in implementing an education and training program that will meet the expectations of the Public Health Service (PHS). This guide was designed to fulfill several purposes. First, it is intended to assist institutional officials and…
Descriptors: Anesthesiology, Animal Caretakers, Animal Facilities, Animal Husbandry
Stecher, Brian M.; Klein, Stephen P. – 1996
In 1992, RAND received a grant from the National Science Foundation to study the technical quality of performance assessments in science and to evaluate their feasibility for use in large-scale testing programs. The specific goals of the project were to assess the reliability and validity of hands-on science testing and to investigate the cost and…
Descriptors: Chemical Reactions, Classification, Elementary Secondary Education, Hands on Science
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Reed, Phil; Doughty, Adam H. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2005
Response rates under random-interval schedules are lower when a brief (500 ms) signal accompanies reinforcement than when there is no signal. The present study examined this signaled-reinforcement effect and its relation to resistance to change. In Experiment 1, rats responded on a multiple random-interval 60-s random-interval 60-s schedule, with…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Operant Conditioning, Intervals, Behavioral Science Research
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Black, Abraham H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1977
Comments on Maier and Seligman's research (EJ 138 911) on learned helplessness, specifically on their analysis of alternatives to the learned helplessness hypothesis. (Author/RK)
Descriptors: Animal Behavior, Critical Thinking, Experimental Psychology, Hypothesis Testing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Myers, Karyn M.; Davis, Michael – Learning & Memory, 2004
The neural mechanisms of fear suppression most commonly are studied through the use of extinction, a behavioral procedure in which a feared stimulus (i.e., one previously paired with shock) is nonreinforced repeatedly, leading to a reduction or elimination of the fear response. Although extinction is perhaps the most convenient index of fear…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Stimuli, Models, Fear
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3